
A research team - Jan Pfänder from the Jean Nicod Institute in Paris and Sacha Altay from the University of Zurich - analyzed data from 67 experimental studies covering nearly 200,000 participants across 40 countries. Most were from the US and Europe, but the results also included data from Asia, Africa, and South America. Participants assessed the credibility of over two thousand news items - both true and false - previously verified by fact-checkers.
Pfänder and Altay published their findings in the article "Spotting false news and doubting true news" in Nature Human Behaviour. The result? In the vast majority of cases, people rated true news as more credible than false news. This effect was consistent regardless of country, headline format, or topic.
Key findings that confirm people’s ability to distinguish truth from falsehood:
- 298 out of 303 analyzed effects showed higher ratings for true information.
- 79.9% of participants had a positive "truth discernment" score.
- The average statistical effect (Cohen’s d = 1.12) is considered large in social sciences.
While concerns about disinformation remain, the researchers emphasize that most of us can distinguish between reliable and false information - at least when asked in an experimental setting.
More doubt toward truth than belief in falsehood
A more interesting and less intuitive result involves the so-called "skepticism bias" - the difference between mistakenly judging true information as false versus mistakenly accepting false information as credible.
Pfänder and Altay found that participants more often rejected true information than believed in false news. This effect was modest (Cohen’s d = 0.32) but consistently present across more than 200 analyzed cases.
Between the results list and the next section, it’s worth noting the context. The authors suggest that this skepticism may stem from how information is presented. Most experiments used Facebook-like layouts - headline, image, short description. According to Reuters Institute research, users trust such content less than news published on editorial websites.
Examples that amplify this effect:
- Headlines with images and leads triggered more skepticism than the same content in plain text.
- When participants saw politically incongruent news (e.g., a liberal article rated by a conservative), they judged it as less credible - regardless of its truth.
Polarization fuels skepticism more than gullibility
Especially interesting are the findings on so-called political congruence. Many studies presented participants with news that aligned or conflicted with their political views. The analysis showed that alignment didn’t affect truth detection - but incongruent news was met with more skepticism.
The table below shows skepticism levels depending on political congruence:
Type of news | Average skepticism level (Cohen’s d) |
---|---|
Aligned with beliefs | −0.20 |
Incongruent with beliefs | +0.58 |
The takeaway? Participants weren’t more likely to believe false news that aligned with their beliefs - but were significantly more skeptical of news that contradicted them.
This is a key insight for communication strategies. Instead of "preaching to the choir", it may be more effective to encourage openness toward information from across the divide.
The bigger problem: distrust in reliable sources
Paradoxically, the real problem isn’t too much trust in fake news - but declining trust in reliable information. As the authors note, since true news is far more common in our environment than falsehoods, rejecting them can have more damaging consequences than occasionally believing disinformation.
Data showing the scale of the issue:
- Only 59% of participants had a positive skepticism score (meaning they more accurately identified falsehoods than truths).
- 35% had a negative discernment score - judging truths as false and vice versa.
- In studies with raw data (over 42,000 participants), only 12% had extremely negative results.
The authors suggest rethinking how we approach educational interventions. Rather than just warning people about fake news, it may be more effective to encourage trust in credible sources. This paradigm shift could improve the quality of public discourse without fostering excessive suspicion.
Relationship and tech. Study about love in the age of likes 👇
A new approach to disinformation
Pfänder and Altay propose a new strategy for combating disinformation. Since most people can distinguish truth from falsehood, the issue is not a lack of skill but a lack of motivation to apply that skill.
They recommend changing the environments in which people consume news - e.g., through reminders to focus on credibility or by redesigning social media platforms. Instead of teaching how to spot fake news, we should encourage reflection and concentration.
This shift from education to motivation may turn out to be the most effective weapon in the fight for better information quality - and greater trust in the truth.
***
The study was conducted by Jan Pfänder (Institut Jean Nicod, École normale supérieure, École des hautes études en sciences sociales, Centre national de la recherche scientifique, Paris) and Sacha Altay (Department of Political Science, University of Zurich). The meta-analysis covered 303 effects from 67 experimental studies involving 194,438 participants from 40 countries. Full results are available at nature.com
COMMERCIAL BREAK
New articles in section Media industry
The most valuable female personal brands in Polish fashion. IMM report
KFi
The ten most popular people in Poland in the "fashion" category generate over 1.5 billion contacts across all types of media in a year. Their value is nearly 400 million zlotys. The ranking was prepared by the Institute of Media Monitoring for "Forbes Women" magazine.
Disinformation ranks above terrorism as global threat
KFi
According to "International Opinion on Global Threats" by Pew Research Center, a median of 72% of adults across 25 countries view the spread of false information online as a major threat to their country. That number places disinformation at the very top of perceived global dangers.
Television 2025. Report by the European Broadcasting Union
Krzysztof Fiedorek
Europeans are watching less and less television. In 2024, the average viewing time was 3 hours and 13 minutes per day. Only 54% of young people had any contact with TV, and public broadcasters held a 23% share. Television is losing relevance, especially among the youngest viewers.
See articles on a similar topic:
Russian Propaganda. Debunk.org Report on Moscow's Disinformation Scale
BARD, PAP Mediaroom
In 2022, the Russian Federation allocated approximately 143 billion rubles to mass media (equivalent to 1.9 billion US dollars), exceeding the planned budget by 25%. For the current year, the Kremlin's budget for this sector is set at 119.2 billion rubles (1.6 billion dollars).
The Deadliest Year for Journalism. 124 Fatalities in 2024
Krzysztof Fiedorek
The year 2024 was the deadliest for media professionals since the Committee to Protect Journalists began tracking these statistics. The tragic figures, published in CPJs latest special report, reached record highs in most monitored categories.
Review of media from around the world. See what they are buzzing about [LINK]
AUTOPROMOCJA Reporterzy.info
What is the media buzzing about? A review of the headlines of the most important newspapers and websites. Events of the day, country, world, media market, economy, sport, foreign media, and even gossip and curiosities. In real time and 24 hours a day. We invite you!
Poles on the Internet. RegionyNEXERY 2024 Report
KFi
The Internet not only connects people but also changes their daily habits in ways that seemed unattainable just a few years ago. Over 40% of Poles work remotely, and IoT devices are gaining popularity in rural areas. The #RegionyNEXERY 2024 report reveals surprising facts about the digital reality.