2.06.2025 Media industry
How do we assess news credibility? Data analysis from 40 countries
Krzysztof Fiedorek

A research team - Jan Pfänder from the Jean Nicod Institute in Paris and Sacha Altay from the University of Zurich - analyzed data from 67 experimental studies covering nearly 200,000 participants across 40 countries. Most were from the US and Europe, but the results also included data from Asia, Africa, and South America. Participants assessed the credibility of over two thousand news items - both true and false - previously verified by fact-checkers.
Pfänder and Altay published their findings in the article "Spotting false news and doubting true news" in Nature Human Behaviour. The result? In the vast majority of cases, people rated true news as more credible than false news. This effect was consistent regardless of country, headline format, or topic.
Key findings that confirm people’s ability to distinguish truth from falsehood:
- 298 out of 303 analyzed effects showed higher ratings for true information.
- 79.9% of participants had a positive "truth discernment" score.
- The average statistical effect (Cohen’s d = 1.12) is considered large in social sciences.
While concerns about disinformation remain, the researchers emphasize that most of us can distinguish between reliable and false information - at least when asked in an experimental setting.
More doubt toward truth than belief in falsehood
A more interesting and less intuitive result involves the so-called "skepticism bias" - the difference between mistakenly judging true information as false versus mistakenly accepting false information as credible.
Pfänder and Altay found that participants more often rejected true information than believed in false news. This effect was modest (Cohen’s d = 0.32) but consistently present across more than 200 analyzed cases.
Between the results list and the next section, it’s worth noting the context. The authors suggest that this skepticism may stem from how information is presented. Most experiments used Facebook-like layouts - headline, image, short description. According to Reuters Institute research, users trust such content less than news published on editorial websites.
Examples that amplify this effect:
- Headlines with images and leads triggered more skepticism than the same content in plain text.
- When participants saw politically incongruent news (e.g., a liberal article rated by a conservative), they judged it as less credible - regardless of its truth.
Polarization fuels skepticism more than gullibility
Especially interesting are the findings on so-called political congruence. Many studies presented participants with news that aligned or conflicted with their political views. The analysis showed that alignment didn’t affect truth detection - but incongruent news was met with more skepticism.
The table below shows skepticism levels depending on political congruence:
Type of news | Average skepticism level (Cohen’s d) |
---|---|
Aligned with beliefs | −0.20 |
Incongruent with beliefs | +0.58 |
The takeaway? Participants weren’t more likely to believe false news that aligned with their beliefs - but were significantly more skeptical of news that contradicted them.
This is a key insight for communication strategies. Instead of "preaching to the choir", it may be more effective to encourage openness toward information from across the divide.
The bigger problem: distrust in reliable sources
Paradoxically, the real problem isn’t too much trust in fake news - but declining trust in reliable information. As the authors note, since true news is far more common in our environment than falsehoods, rejecting them can have more damaging consequences than occasionally believing disinformation.
Data showing the scale of the issue:
- Only 59% of participants had a positive skepticism score (meaning they more accurately identified falsehoods than truths).
- 35% had a negative discernment score - judging truths as false and vice versa.
- In studies with raw data (over 42,000 participants), only 12% had extremely negative results.
The authors suggest rethinking how we approach educational interventions. Rather than just warning people about fake news, it may be more effective to encourage trust in credible sources. This paradigm shift could improve the quality of public discourse without fostering excessive suspicion.
A new approach to disinformation
Pfänder and Altay propose a new strategy for combating disinformation. Since most people can distinguish truth from falsehood, the issue is not a lack of skill but a lack of motivation to apply that skill.
They recommend changing the environments in which people consume news - e.g., through reminders to focus on credibility or by redesigning social media platforms. Instead of teaching how to spot fake news, we should encourage reflection and concentration.
This shift from education to motivation may turn out to be the most effective weapon in the fight for better information quality - and greater trust in the truth.
***
The study was conducted by Jan Pfänder (Institut Jean Nicod, École normale supérieure, École des hautes études en sciences sociales, Centre national de la recherche scientifique, Paris) and Sacha Altay (Department of Political Science, University of Zurich). The meta-analysis covered 303 effects from 67 experimental studies involving 194,438 participants from 40 countries. Full results are available at nature.com
COMMERCIAL BREAK
New articles in section Media industry
Trust in social media. Youtube beats TikTok and X
Krzysztof Fiedorek
Do we really trust social media? A new study reveals major differences in how top platforms are rated. Trust goes where there's authenticity, not just algorithms. The role of people is growing while brand influence is fading.
Artificial intelligence in newsrooms. Three realities of the AI era in media
Krzysztof Fiedorek
According to a report by the European Broadcasting Union, many newsrooms already use AI but still do not fully trust it. Audiences do not want "robotic" news, and the technologies themselves though fast can be costly, unreliable, and surprisingly human in their mistakes.
Zero-click search 2025. The even bigger end of clicking in search engines
Bartłomiej Dwornik
Google is giving up its role as a web signpost. More and more, it wants to be the destination of the whole journey. ChatGPT and Perplexity are hot on its heels, changing the rules of the search game. AI Overviews is a card from the same deck. Only content creators are losing ground in this race.
See articles on a similar topic:
Can a Robot Be Good Boss? Researchers from SWPS Look for Answers
SWPS
A robot giving orders at work is no longer a science fiction scenario - it's a research topic. Scientists from SWPS University in Poland set out to find out whether a robot can effectively manage human workers.
Mass Media in Poland
Agnieszka Osińska
Into the decade of years 90. Poland entered with media national, subjected to the political control. Starting from the half of 1989 our media market underwent transformations, biggest probably from the time of the World War II.
Blogs in E-commerce. Report by Elephate and Senuto
KrzysztoF
The average number of indexed articles on e-commerce blogs is 565, with each post attracting 347 readers per month. The health industry generates the highest organic traffic per single article. The authors of the "E-commerce Blog Ranking" take a behind-the-scenes look at the commercial blogosphere.
Milgram Experiment 2023. AI Can Encourage Violence
KrzysztoF
Researchers from SWPS University replicated the famous Milgram experiment, in which participants were instructed to inflict pain on another person under the authority’s command. This time, the authority was a robot. It’s the first study showing that people are willing to harm another person when a robot commands them to do so.